Intended for healthcare professionals

Research

Universal parenting programme to prevent early childhood behavioural problems: cluster randomised trial

BMJ 2008; 336 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.39451.609676.AE (Published 07 February 2008) Cite this as: BMJ 2008;336:318
  1. Harriet Hiscock, postdoctoral research fellow and paediatrician 123,
  2. Jordana K Bayer, postdoctoral research fellow and clinical child psychologist 123,
  3. Anna Price, research assistant13,
  4. Obioha C Ukoumunne, postdoctoral research fellow and statistician234,
  5. Susan Rogers, clinical research fellow 5,
  6. Melissa Wake, paediatrician and senior research fellow123
  1. 1Centre for Community Child Health, Royal Children’s Hospital, Parkville VIC 3052, Australia
  2. 2Department of Paediatrics, University of Melbourne, Parkville
  3. 3Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Parkville
  4. 4Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics Unit, Royal Children’s Hospital, Parkville
  5. 5Parenting Research Centre, Carlton, VIC 3053
  1. Correspondence to: H Hiscock harriet.hiscock{at}rch.org.au
  • Accepted 21 November 2007

Abstract

Objective To determine whether a parenting programme, offered universally in primary care, can prevent behavioural problems in children and improve parenting and maternal mental health.

Design Cluster randomised trial.

Setting 40 primary care nursing centres (clusters) in Victoria, Australia.

Participants 733 English speaking mothers of 8 month old children sequentially recruited from well child appointments; 656 retained at 24 months.

Intervention Structured three session programme at age 8-15 months, co-led by well child providers and a parenting expert. The programme covered normal development and behaviour, strategies to increase desired behaviour, and strategies to reduce unwanted behaviour.

Main outcome measures Maternal report of child externalising behaviour (child behavior checklist 1½-5 year old), parenting (parent behavior checklist), and maternal mental health (depression anxiety stress scales) at 18 and 24 months.

Results At 18 months, child behaviour and parenting scores were similar in the two groups. At 24 months, externalising scores in the intervention and control groups were similar (mean 11.9 (SD 7.2) v 12.9 (7.4)); however, on the parent behavior checklist subscale scores, intervention group parents were less likely to report harsh/abusive parenting (mean 38.9 (SD 7.7) v 40.5 (8.8); adjusted mean difference −1.83, 95% confidence interval −3.12 to −0.55) and unreasonable expectations of child development (40.9 (9.9) v 42.7 (9.6); −2.18, −3.74 to −0.62). Mean scores for nurturing parenting and maternal mental health were similar in the two groups at both times.

Conclusions A universal parenting programme resulted in modest improvement in parenting factors that predict behavioural problems in children but did not reduce externalising behavioural problems or affect maternal mental health at 2 years.

Trial registration ISRCTN 77531789.

Introduction

Behavioural problems affect approximately one in seven 4-17 year olds in Australia and internationally and have major personal, societal, and economic ramifications.1 2 3 Behavioural problems in children are primarily grouped as externalising (such as oppositional defiance, aggression) and internalising (such as anxiety/depression, withdrawal) problems.1 Left untreated, up to 50% of behavioural problems in preschool children develop into later mental health problems, including oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and depression.4 In addition to the substantial costs of treating such problems, social costs include increased school dropout and subsequent unemployment, family stress and break down, increased drug and alcohol misuse, and increased crime.2

Possible approaches to reducing childhood mental health problems include management of established problems, secondary prevention targeting children with either family/social risk factors or early signs of disorder, and universal prevention offered to all children.5 Management approaches include drug treatment, individual psychotherapy, and parenting programmes, the last of which have the strongest evidence of efficacy to date.2 6 7 8 Parenting programmes share the goals of modifying the key aspects of parenting known to contribute to behavioural problems in children, by reducing harsh or abusive parenting, increasing warm parenting, and informing parental knowledge of normal development.2 6 7 9 Although effective, parenting programmes are costly and time intensive, require an available workforce trained in delivery of evidence based treatments, and are currently accessible to few children.1 Given that treatment services can never hope to meet the needs of all children with mental health problems, prevention is an essential first step in a public health approach that would also include adequate treatment services.10

Several high quality randomised trials have now reported on targeted approaches for high risk families. Substantial effect sizes have been reported for approaches targeting children with early behavioural problems: 0.3-0.7 for reductions in parenting risks and 0.5-0.9 for reductions in behavioural problems.11 12 13 Approaches targeting children with family or social risk factors also seem to be effective but have targeted very restricted populations (very disadvantaged young mothers, families eligible for food assistance, and socioeconomically disadvantaged areas).11 14 15 16 17 18

However, all targeted approaches share five possible limitations: misclassification of children on the basis of early risk factors, as at most half of the children who later develop mental health problems have identifiable risk factors and are therefore likely to be targeted by such programmes10 19; stigmatisation, which can lead to poor uptake rates20; delivery to only the minority at highest risk, when most children with subsequent mental health problems come from the numerically much larger “low risk” population10; lack of denominator figures in published studies that are critical to enable estimation of population risk reduction and thus determination of impact at the population level; and a tendency to offer targeted programmes to children of preschool or primary school age, by which time their behaviour may already have had substantial and lasting negative effects.

Universal prevention offered to all families lacks these limitations.5 However, no high quality evidence showing the effectiveness of universal prevention has been published.2 6 Here, we report intermediate trial outcomes of the first truly universal parenting programme aiming to prevent early childhood externalising behaviours before onset. As previously reported,21 programme content was informed by a review of the literature on intervention (social learning and attachment theoretical perspectives) and developed in partnership with parenting experts and well child providers. We designed the programme to be suitable for all parents and to be delivered by trained health professionals working in the primary care setting. We hypothesised that families receiving the intervention would report fewer childhood externalising behavioural problems; less harsh discipline, more nurturing parenting, and more reasonable expectations of normal child development; and fewer symptoms of maternal depression or anxiety. We powered the study to be able to detect much smaller effect sizes than those reported for targeted approaches, given that the entire population is the denominator.

Methods

Setting and participants

We ran the trial in greater Melbourne (population 3.4 million), Victoria, Australia. We ranked Melbourne’s 31 local government areas by the census derived socioeconomic indexes for areas index of relative disadvantage22; we then divided them into thirds representing low, middle, and high socioeconomic status. From each third, we selected two local government areas to provide a broad spread of social circumstance; all maternal and child health nurses in these six local government areas participated.

In Victoria, free health visits are scheduled at 1, 2, 4, 6-8, 12, 18, 24, and 42 months of age, and more than 90% of all parents attend visits during the first six months.23 To access a key developmental transition associated with an abrupt rise in parenting challenges (that is, the point at which infants become mobile), maternal and child health nurses consecutively invited mothers of 6-7 month old infants attending in August/September 2004 to take part in the toddlers without tears study. Mothers with insufficient English to complete questionnaires were excluded. The research team telephoned interested mothers and mailed the baseline questionnaire to be returned with written informed consent.

Universal intervention

The intervention consisted of three sessions targeting key modifiable parenting risk factors for childhood behavioural problems: unreasonable expectations, harsh parenting, and lack of nurturing parenting.2 7 9 At the routine 8 month visit, mothers received four handouts discussing normal child behavioural, motor, and social development over the ensuing 12 months and ways to encourage language development. At 12 months, parents attended a two hour group session discussing ways to develop a warm and sensitive relationship with their toddler and to plan for and encourage desirable behaviour in toddlers. The session introduced the need to plan ahead for “difficult” situations in which toddlers are likely to misbehave and offered alternatives to common “irrational beliefs” by parents that can lead to harsh parenting. At 15 months, parents attended another two hour group session discussing ways to manage unwanted behaviour in children. Rather than smacking and yelling, parents were encouraged to identify “low priority” problem behaviours (for which strategies such as planned ignoring, distraction, and logical choices were discussed) and “high priority” behaviours (for which “quiet time” was discussed). All sessions took place at a local maternal and child health centre and were delivered by a nurse and a co-facilitator expert in running parenting groups. Programme content drew on attachment theory (warm, sensitive parent-child interactions) and social learning theory (reinforcing desired behaviours and extinguishing undesirable behaviours).

Intervention nurses attended a half hour training session (for the 8 month session) and two 2.5 hour training sessions (for the 12 and 15 month sessions) led by HH (paediatrician) and JB (child psychologist) and underpinned by a programme manual and written handouts for parents. Training incorporated didactic teaching, written information, role play, and video vignettes of appropriate parenting responses to common childhood behaviours. The standardised programme manual and training encouraged fidelity to the programme, and nurses completed a checklist at the end of each group session to record components of the intervention delivered.

Usual care

Families in the control arm received usual care from their maternal and child health centre, which may include advice on children’s behaviour but does not include a structured, evidence based parenting programme for early childhood behaviour.

Measures

Mothers completed written questionnaires at 7 months (baseline sociodemographic details, maternal mental health, and family stress), 12 months (baseline parenting style and partner relationship), and 18 and 24 months (outcomes). The primary outcome was externalising behavioural problems measured with the 99 item validated child behavior checklist 1½-5 year old.24 The checklist also quantifies internalising behavioural problems and yields both raw scores (used in this report to compare groups as our primary outcome) and T scores with a clinical cut point derived from the combined norming sample of children aged 1-5 years (used in this report to describe our sample relative to international norms). Another validated outcome measure was the parent behavior checklist,9 a 32 item measure of three parenting styles known to contribute to childhood behavioural problems (harsh discipline, nurturing parenting, and expectations of normal development) for parents of preschool (1-5 years) children. The parent behavior checklist yields T scores (mean 50, SD 10) derived from norms with 6 month age bands but no clinical cut points. We measured maternal mental health with the depression anxiety stress scales,25 a 21 item measure that, as well as mean scores for each subscale, has cut points to indicate risk of depression (score >9), anxiety (score >7), and stress (score >14). We measured children’s temperament, a potential confounder of the effect of the parenting programme, with a parent rated global temperament item (1=“much easier than average” to 5=“much more difficult than average”).26 Sociodemographic factors included infants’ sex and birth order, maternal age, marital status, education, and language mainly spoken at home. We assigned each family an Australian index of disadvantage score based on the postal code of their home address (72 separate postcodes in the six local government areas, index of disadvantage mean 1044.5, SD 68.9, range 825-1140).22

Randomisation

We ran the trial in accordance with the CONSORT statement for cluster randomised trials.27 We randomised maternal and child health centres (clusters) in the study to minimise the contamination that could occur when participants attending the same centre are randomised to different groups. After recruitment was complete, an independent statistician randomly allocated maternal and child health centres (and therefore their families), stratified by local government area, to intervention or control arms. Within each of the local government areas, a list of participating centres was created. As we considered balance on socioeconomic status to be important, randomisation was done with a computer generated allocation sequence by matching pairs of centres according to the closeness of their average socioeconomic disadvantage scores and then randomising one centre from each pair to the intervention arm. Masking occurred at randomisation, with group allocation concealed from researchers and participants until allocation was complete.

Sample size

To detect a reduction of 0.3 standard deviation units in the mean score for the primary outcomes with 80% power at the two sided significance level of 0.05, 175 children would be needed for each of the intervention and control groups. We inflated this sample size by a design effect of 1.18 to 210 infants per trial arm to allow for correlation between responses within the same cluster (that is, maternal and child health centre),28 assuming an average cluster size of seven (the number of eligible mothers attending each centre) and an intracluster correlation coefficient of 0.03. Assuming a 30% dropout rate, the study needed a final sample size of 300 infants in each trial arm.

Analyses

We analysed the trial arms as randomised at the level of the individual, applying the intention to treat principle. We implemented unadjusted and adjusted analyses (potential prognostic factors listed in table 2) of the outcomes (all quantitative) by using random effects linear regression models fitted by maximum likelihood estimation to allow for the correlation between the responses of participants from the same maternal and child health centre.29 We present means and standard deviations for each trial arm, along with the mean difference between arms, 95% confidence intervals, and P values. We calculated effect sizes by dividing the mean difference between the trial arms by the pooled standard deviation. We used the bootstrap method to validate analyses of all skewed outcomes.30 We used Stata 9.2 for the analyses.

Results

Of the 1069 eligible families, 840 expressed interest and 733 (69%) completed the baseline questionnaire (figure). Non-participating families were more likely than participating families to have low index of disadvantage scores (mean 1033.7 (SD 74.6), range 825-1132, P=0.03), but sex of child was similar in participating and non-participating families. Table 1 describes characteristics of the sample. At 18 months, 299 (91%) intervention families and 373 (92%) control families returned their questionnaires; this fell to 293 (89%) and 363 (90%) at 24 months. All families returned at least one questionnaire. Families who completed the follow-up questionnaires were representative of the baseline sample with respect to sex and temperament of children, maternal education and mental health, and household income and index of disadvantage score.

Figure1

Flow chart of participants. *Take-up of intervention was voluntary: 246 families reported receiving intervention. †All lost to follow-up owing to failure to return questionnaires

Table 1

 Baseline characteristics by trial arm. Values are numbers (percentages) unless stated otherwise

View this table:

Programme fidelity

Content checklists completed by nurses indicated that the full programme was delivered for 95% of group sessions. In the intervention arm, 307 (93%) families attended the 8 month visit, 222 (67%) families attended a 12 month group, 185 (56%) attended a 15 month group, and 160 (49%) attended both groups. Primary reasons offered when parents missed group sessions were “family member sick” (52 families), “too busy” (28 families), and “no childcare” (27 families). On average, the number of parents at each group session was seven (range 2-12). The research team mailed the written handouts to all intervention families who missed groups at the maternal and child health centres, 98% of whom subsequently reported receiving the handouts.

Childhood behaviour

Compared with US externalising T score norms for 1-5 year olds (mean 50 (SD 10); 10% above clinical cut point), our sample of toddlers had slightly lower externalising problems at 18 months (mean 49.0 (SD 8.9); 4.5% above clinical cut point) and 24 months (49.5 (9.4); 6%). Internalising problems were also lower.

The mean (raw) externalising behaviour scores were similar in the intervention and control groups at 18 months (mean 12.1 (SD 6.9) v 11.9 (6.8); adjusted mean difference 0.16, 95% confidence interval −1.01 to 1.33; P=0.79) and 24 months (mean 11.9 (SD 7.2) v 12.9 (7.4); adjusted mean difference −0.79, −2.27 to 0.69; P=0.30). Mean internalising scores were also similar in the two groups at both 18 and 24 months (table 2).

Table 2

 Main outcome comparisons

View this table:

Parenting

At 18 months, mean harsh discipline and unreasonable developmental expectations scores were similar in intervention families and control families. By 24 months, intervention mothers reported less harsh discipline and unreasonable expectations with their toddlers than did control mothers. For the harsh discipline subscale, the adjusted mean difference of −1.83 (95% confidence interval −3.12 to −0.55; P=0.005) corresponds to an effect size of −0.22 (95% confidence interval −0.38 to −0.07). For the unreasonable developmental expectations subscale, the adjusted mean difference of −2.18 (−3.74 to −0.62; P=0.006) corresponds to an effect size of −0.22 (−0.38 to −0.06). Mean scores for nurturing parenting were similar in the two groups at both 18 and 24 months (table 2).

Maternal mental health

The mean maternal depression, anxiety, and stress subscale scores were not markedly different between the two groups at either 18 or 24 months (table 2).

Discussion

A universal parenting programme, designed to prevent early childhood behavioural problems, resulted in modest reductions in maternal report at 24 months of parenting risk factors known to predict such problems.2 6 7 9 Although the intervention reduced harsh parental discipline and inappropriate developmental expectations, it did not lead to more nurturing parenting (which was high in both groups). The programme was acceptable to parents, was feasible in a routine primary care setting, and achieved greater reach than other reported programmes. However, we showed no significant impact on externalising behavioural problems in 2 year olds or on maternal mental health.

Strengths

The study had several strengths. Firstly, a 69% uptake on the basis of eligible birth records is high for population trials.5 Non-participants’ index of disadvantage scores were only marginally lower than those of participants, indicating that our results could be generalised to a wide socioeconomic spectrum, although perhaps not the lowest end. Secondly, we used well validated parent reported outcome measures appropriate to this age group. Thirdly, all clusters and more than 85% of all families remained in the trial, strongly supporting both the validity of the findings and the acceptability of the approach.

Limitations

We included single source (maternal report) rather than multi-source assessment, and the results could thus be open to bias as mothers could not be blinded to their group membership. Although primary caregiver report is recognised as the most useful measure of early childhood behaviour,31 multi-source assessment, including paternal report, could assess the family environment and childhood behavioural outcomes more extensively. However, direct observational measures are generally impractical for population based studies of this size,32 and, because many children are not yet attending out of home care on a regular basis, the value of the day care provider as a third informant is limited at 2 years of age.

A second limitation is that higher risk families may have preferentially not participated in the trial. This would apply particularly to non-English speaking parents, as we do not know the extent to which the 31% of eligible families who did not join the trial were hard to reach, high risk families as opposed to capable, experienced families who rightly saw no need for the programme. Nevertheless, we successfully recruited many families with known risk factors for behavioural problems; 39% of the final sample were exposed to clinical levels of maternal depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms; substance misuse; partner conflict; social isolation; or financial problems.5

Thirdly, although almost all families received some of the programme, only 49% received the full programme, underscoring the difficulty of implementing a true population prevention programme. If the parenting programme became part of routine primary care, the dosage received by families could conceivably increase (owing to streamlining and promotion of local sessions) but equally could further decrease. Fourthly, we report here only short term outcomes, but interventions for externalising behaviour may have “sleeper” effects,33 the detection of which would require longer term follow-up.33

Meaning of the study

This is the first trial to evaluate a parenting programme delivered with a truly universal, as opposed to high risk, sample involving the full spectrum of social advantage through disadvantage. Previous prevention trials targeting existing behavioural problems reported parenting effect sizes ranging from 0.3 to 0.7. Our effect size on parenting behaviours of 0.2, although modest, is therefore promising, given the brevity of this universal prevention programme that started in infancy before the onset of any behavioural difficulties. However, this would only be of value if flow-on effects were shown at a population level at 3 years or beyond.

In conclusion, the outcomes at 2 years are insufficient to support widespread introduction of a very early universal programme to prevent behavioural problems in toddlers. If additional, longer term benefits emerge by preschool age, such universal prevention could be considered as a component of population childhood mental health strategies, in conjunction with effective targeted prevention and clinical treatment programmes.

What is already known on this topic

  • Behavioural problems affect up to 20% of children and have cumulative personal, societal, and economic ramifications

  • Prevention targeted to high risk families can be effective but has limited population reach and may stigmatise recipients

What this study adds

  • By age 24 months, a brief, universally offered parenting programme led to modest improvements in parenting risks known to contribute to child behavioural problems but did not improve maternal distress or toddlers’ behaviour

  • Longer term follow-up is needed to determine effectiveness in preventing preschool behavioural problems

Footnotes

  • We thank maternal and child health nurses and families of the Melbourne cities of Glen Eira, Greater Geelong, Kingston, Maribyrnong, Nillumbik, and Stonnington who took part in this research and the co-facilitators from the Parenting Research Centre.

  • Contributors: HH, JKB, and MW were involved in the conception and design of the study and obtained funding. HH, JKB, SR, and AP were involved with acquisition of study data and, together with OCU and MW, were responsible for analysis and interpretation of the data. OCU did the statistical analysis with assistance from AP. HH and JKB drafted the manuscript with critical revision from OCU, MW, AP, and SR. HH is the guarantor.

  • Funding: This project was funded by the Telstra Community Development Fund. The authors’ work was independent of the funders. HH’s and OCU’s postdoctoral positions are funded by Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Population Health Capacity Building Grant 436914; MW is part funded by NHMRC Career Development Award 284556; and JB is part funded by an Australian Rotary Health Research Fund Postdoctoral Fellowship.

  • Competing interests: None declared.

  • Ethical approval: Ethics in Human Research Committee of the Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia (project approval 24020A).

References

View Abstract