Skip to main content
Log in

A randomized comparison of patients’ understanding of number needed to treat and other common risk reduction formats

  • Original Articles
  • Published:
Journal of General Internal Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

BACKGROUND: Commentators have suggested that patients may understand quantitative information about treatment benefits better when they are presented as numbers needed to treat (NNT) rather than as absolute or relative risk reductions.

OBJECTIVE: To determine whether NNT helps patients interpret treatment benefits better than absolute risk reduction (ARR), relative risk reduction (RRR), or a combination of all three of these risk reduction presentations (COMBO).

DESIGN: Randomized cross-sectional survey.

SETTING: University internal medicine clinic.

PATIENTS: Three hundred fifty-seven men and women, ages 50 to 80, who presented for health care.

INTERVENTIONS: Subjects were given written information about the baseline risk of a hypothetical “disease Y” and were asked (1) to compare the benefits of two drug treatments for disease Y, stating which provided more benefit; and (2) to calculate the effect of one of those drug treatments on a given baseline risk of disease. Risk information was presented to each subject in one of four randomly allocated risk formats: NNT, ARR, RRR, or COMBO.

MAIN RESULTS: When asked to state which of two treatments provided more benefit, subjects who received the RRR format responded correctly most often (60% correct vs 43% for COMBO, 42% for ARR, and 30% for NNT, P=.001). Most subjects were unable to calculate the effect of drug treatment on the given baseline risk of disease, although subjects receiving the RRR and ARR formats responded correctly more often (21% and 17% compared to 7% for COMBO and 6% for NNT, P=.004).

CONCLUSION: Patients are best able to interpret the benefits of treatment when they are presented in an RRR format with a given baseline risk of disease. ARR also is easily interpreted. NNT is often misinterpreted by patients and should not be used alone to communicate risk to patients.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Institute of Medicine (US). Envisioning the National Health Care Quality Report. Washington, DC: The Institute; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Rajkumar S, Sampathkumar P, Gustafson A. Number needed to treat is a simple measure of treatment efficacy for clinicians. J Gen Intern Med. 1996;11:357–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Schwartz LM, Woloshin S, Black WC, Welch HG. The role of numberacy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography. Ann Intern Med. 1997;127:966–72.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Laupacis A, Sackett D, Roberts R. An assessment of clinically useful measures of the consequences of treatment. N Engl J Med. 1988;318:1728–33.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Sackett D. Inference and decision at the bedside. J Clin Epidemiol. 1989;42:309–16.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Hutton JL. Number needed to treat: properties and problems. J Royal Statist Soc A. 2000;163:403–19.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Hux JE, Naylor CD. Communicating the benefits of chronic preventive therapy: does the format of efficacy data determine patients’ acceptance of treatment? Med Decis Making. 1995;15:152–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Grimes DA, Snively GR. Patients’ understanding of medical risks: implications for genetic counseling. Obstet Gynecol. 1999;93:910–4.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Woloshin S, Schwartz L, Byram S, Fischhoff B, Welch H. A new scale for assessing perceptions of chance: a validation study. Med Decis Making. 2000;20:298–307.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Lipkus I, Samsa G, Rimer B. General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples. Med Decis Making. 2001;21:37–44.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Woloshin S, Schwartz LM. How can we help people make sense of medical data? Eff Clin Prac. 1999;2:176–83.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Feldman-Stewart D, Kocovski N, McConnell BA, Brundage MD, Mackillop WJ. Perception of quantitative information for treatment decisions. Med Decis Making. 2000;20:228–38.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. McAlister F, Straus S, Guyatt G, Haynes RB, Users’ guide to the medical literature XX. Integrating research evidence with the care of the individual patient. J Am Med Assoc. 2000;283:2829–36.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Rothman A, Salovey P, Antone C, Keough K, Martin CD. The influence of message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. J Exp Social Psychol. 1993;29:408–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Bobbio M, Demichelis B, Giustetto G. Completeness of reporting trial results: effect on physicians’ willingness to prescribe. Lancet. 1994;343:1209–11.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Cranney M, Walley T. Same information, different decisions: the influence of evidence on the management of hypertension in the elderly. Br J Gen Pract. 1996;46:661–3.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Forrow L, Taylor WC, Arnold RM. Absolutely relative: how research results are summarized can affect treatment decisions. Am J Med. 1992;92:121–4.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Bucher HC, Weinbacher M, Gyr K. Influence of method of reporting study results on decision of physicians to prescribe drugs to lower cholesterol concentration. BMJ. 1994;309:761–4.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Naylor CD, Chen E, Strauss B. Measured enthusiasm: does the method of reporting trial results alter perceptions of therapeutic effectiveness? Ann Intern Med. 1992;117:916–21.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Nikolajevic-Sarunac J, Henry DA, O’Connell DL, Robertson J. Effects of information framing on the intentions of family physicians to prescribe long-term hormone replacement therapy. J Gen Intern Med. 1999;14:591–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. National Center for Education Statistics (US). Adult literacy in America: a first look at the findings of the national adult literacy survey. Washington, DC: US Department of Education; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stacey L. Sheridan MD, MPH.

Additional information

Conference Presentation: Society of General Internal Medicine Conference, San Diego, Calif, May 2001. Awarded the Mack Lipkin, Sr. Award for an outstanding presentation by an associate member.

Financial support: Dr. Sheridan was supported by a National Research Services Award (Public Health Service Grant #PE 14001-14). Drs. Pignone and Lewis were supported by the Lineberger Comprehensive Cancer Center and American Cancer Society Career Development Awards (#01-195-01 and #00-180-00).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sheridan, S.L., Pignone, M.P. & Lewis, C.L. A randomized comparison of patients’ understanding of number needed to treat and other common risk reduction formats. J GEN INTERN MED 18, 884–892 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2003.21102.x

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2003.21102.x

Key words

Navigation