Elsevier

Neurotoxicology and Teratology

Volume 26, Issue 3, May–June 2004, Pages 359-371
Neurotoxicology and Teratology

Neurodevelopmental effects of postnatal lead exposure at very low levels

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2004.01.010Get rights and content

Abstract

This study is among the first to examine specific neurobehavioral deficits in children exposed at very low lead levels. A systematic analysis for the presence of a threshold of lead exposure was conducted. The sample consisted of 246 African American, inner-city children from whom blood lead concentrations were assessed at 7.5 years of age. The results consistently show neurobehavioral deficits in relation to low levels of lead in the areas of intelligence, reaction time, visual–motor integration, fine motor skills, attention, including executive function, off-task behaviors, and teacher-reported withdrawn behaviors. Effects were identified in the specific domains of attention, executive function, visual–motor integration, social behavior, and motor skills, which have been previously suggested as part of lead's “behavioral signature”. Visual inspection of nonparametric regression plots suggested a gradual linear dose–response relation for most endpoints. No threshold discontinuity was evident. Regression analyses in which lead exposure was dichotomized at 10 μg/dl were no more likely to be significant than analyses dichotomizing exposure at 5 μg/dl. Given that associations were found between lead levels as low as 3 μg/dl for multiple outcomes, these data provide additional evidence that there is no apparent lower bound threshold for postnatal lead exposure.

Introduction

In 1995, the World Health Organization [72] reviewed the extensive literature on postnatal lead exposure and children's neurobehavioral development and determined that the effects of lead are credible and persistent, and there appears to be no safe threshold for lead exposure. Bellinger, Dietrich, and others have proposed that the principal endpoints affected by lead exposure (“behavioral signature”) are attention, executive function, visual–motor reasoning skills, vestibular–proprioceptive control and social behavior, and Lanphear et al. have concluded that there is no safe threshold [46]. Thus, Lanphear et al. have reported significant associations where poorer cognitive performance was evident at exposure levels lower than 5 μg/dl. Despite these reports, most government agencies have continued to use 10 μg/dl as a criterion in public health advisories. For example, the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency [25] and the Center for Disease Control [14] recommend that lead levels above 10 μg/dl be avoided. As a result of extensive efforts to reduce lead levels in the environment, mean lead level in children has decreased from 15 μg/dl in the 1970s to 4 μg/dl in the 1990s [7]. Nevertheless, many children continue to be exposed above the recommended “avoid” level. For example, an assessment of Mexican-American children showed that approximately 5% of children of all ages still have lead levels above 10 μg/dl [57].

Because postnatal lead exposure is often associated with socioenvironmentally more disadvantaged homes, researchers have assessed a broad range of control variables in an attempt to distinguish the effects of lead exposure from other socioenvironmental influences. Confounding variables have been assessed so comprehensively that it has been argued that some studies have “overcontrolled” for the social environment, misattributing lead effects to the environment [3]. Even so, after controlling statistically for these confounding effects, most studies continue to find associations of lead exposure with IQ [9], [21], [30], [46], [49], [66] and visual–motor integration [19], [30], [31]. Significant associations have also been reported in the areas of achievement, including reading [27], [29], [55], [75], math, and spelling [9], [11], [28], [75]. Postnatal lead exposure has also been found to be associated with deficits in motor skills (specifically, speed and dexterity), memory abilities, advanced spatial functions and attention [20], [26], and slower reaction times in laboratory assessments [51], [55], [73], [74].

Attention is one domain that has been examined in particular detail [10], [11], [31], [46], [63], [76]. Bellinger et al. [10] attempted to identify the specific aspects of attention that are affected by exposure to lead. Using a middle-class sample whose mean blood lead levels were reported as low (79.2% of the sample was below 5 μg/dl), Bellinger et al. administered Mirsky's attention battery, which assesses sustained attention, focused attention, shift (executive function), and encoding (working memory). Significant associations were found solely in the domains of focus and executive function.

In the past few years, increased attention has been directed to examining behavior problems in lead exposed children. Byers and Lord [12] had noted that heavily exposed children are distractible, impulsive, and aggressive. More recently, antisocial, delinquent, and aggressive behaviors have also been reported in children exposed to low levels of lead [11], [23], [56], even in very young children. In a study of 1- to 3-year-olds, whose mean lead level was 10.4 μg/dl, Mendelsohn et al. [50] found that children exposed to lead had more difficulties with emotional regulation and behavior orientation. These children were more hyperactive, impulsive, and easily frustrated, more withdrawn and lacking in interest.

Although lead exposure has been linked to a broad range of neurobehavioral endpoints, the domains affected are not always consistent across studies. Faust and Brown [26] did not find consistent results on the Achenbach's Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL [1]), the measure used to identify aggressive and delinquent children in the studies cited previously. Silva et al. [63] found no association with intelligence. Needleman et al. [56] found no relation with reaction time and found associations only in the vigilance domain, but not the focused and executive function factors on Mirsky's attention battery reported by Bellinger et al. [10].

Our study examined postnatal lead exposure in a sample of children with very low levels of exposure (mean=5.4 μg/dl). The levels are markedly lower than in earlier low-level lead studies [e.g., Bellinger et al. [10] (mean=10 μg/dl); Dietrich et al. [20] (mean=11.8 μg/dl, for similar-aged children); McMichael et al. [48] (mean=19 μg/dl); Winneke et al. [74] (mean=8.2 μg/dl)] and slightly lower than some of the more recent studies of very low postnatal lead exposure [Bellinger et al. [9] (mean=6.5 μg/dl); Fergusson et al. [28] (mean=6.2 μg/dl)]. To date, only one study has reported levels lower than those presented in this research [Lanphear et al. [46] (geometric mean=1.9 μg/dl)]. In our study, we examined the specific domains where deficits have been previously identified to determine similarities and differences compared with the findings reported in previous studies. In addition, we attempted to identify a lower bound threshold for lead exposure, using nonparametric regression, in several cognitive and behavioral domains where the effects of lead have been shown to be harmful.

Section snippets

Sample

The sample consisted of 237 African American, inner-city children for whom blood lead levels were obtained while they were participating in a larger study (N=337) on the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on child development [37], [38], [39], [40]. Their mothers had been recruited at their first visit (mean=23.4 weeks of gestation, S.D.=8.2) to the prenatal clinic of a large, inner-city maternity hospital serving primarily (92%) African American women. Each mother was interviewed regarding

Sample characteristics

The sample was predominantly lower class with more than 68% receiving some form of public assistance (Table 1). Only 15.9% of the children's primary caregivers were married. The majority of the children (86.5%) were raised by their biological mothers; while 2.5% were raised by their fathers, 8% by a grandparent, 2.1% by an aunt, and fewer than 1% by an adoptive parent. In addition, 19% of the children were cared for by a caregiver other than their parent for at least 4 months. The

Discussion

The results of this study show consistent neurobehavioral deficits in relation to low levels of lead exposure in a lower SES, urban sample. These deficits were found in the domains of overall IQ, performance IQ, reaction time, visual–motor integration, fine motor skills, attention including executive function, off-task behaviors, and withdrawn behaviors on the TRF. Although at least one other study [46] has found deficits in children exposed to levels less than 5 μg/dl, the research presented

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by grants R01-AA06966 and R01-AA09524 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, with supplemental support from a Minority Access to Research Careers grant T34-GM08030 and a Minority Biomedical Research Support grant S06-RR08167 from the National Institutes of Health, and a grant from the Joseph Young, Sr., Fund from the State of Michigan. We would like to thank Robert J. Sokol and Susan S. Martier for their collaboration in the recruitment of this

References (76)

  • T.M Achenbach

    Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile

    (1991)
  • E.C Banks et al.

    Effects of low level lead exposure on cognitive function in children: a review of behavioral neuropsychological and biological evidence

    Neurotoxicology

    (1997)
  • R.A Barkley

    Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

    (1990)
  • A.T Beck et al.

    Beck Depression Inventory Manual

    (1987)
  • K.E Beery

    Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration

    (1989)
  • D Bellinger

    Interpreting the literature on lead and child development: the neglected role of the “experimental system”

    Neurotoxicol. Teratol.

    (1995)
  • D Bellinger et al.

    Low-level lead exposure and cognitive function in children

    Pediatr. Ann.

    (1994)
  • D Bellinger et al.

    Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement: a long-term follow up study

    Pediatrics

    (1992)
  • D Bellinger et al.

    Attentional correlates of dentin and bone lead levels in adolescents

    Arch. Environ. Health

    (1994)
  • D Bellinger et al.

    Pre- and postnatal lead exposure and behavior problems in school-aged children

    Environ. Res.

    (1994)
  • R.K Byers et al.

    Late effects of lead poisoning on mental development

    Am. J. Dis. Child.

    (1943)
  • B.M Caldwell et al.

    Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment

    (1979)
  • CDC

    Preventing Lead Poisoning in Young Children: A Statement by the Center for Disease Control

    (1991)
  • R.D Coddington

    Measuring the stressfulness of a child's environment

  • E DeRenzi et al.

    Verbal and nonverbal short-term memory impairment following hemispheric damage

    Cortex

    (1975)
  • L.R Derogatis

    SCL-90-R administration. Scoring and procedures manual II for the revised version and other instruments of the psychopathology rating scale series

  • K.N Dietrich

    Environmental neurotoxicants and psychological development

  • K.N Dietrich et al.

    Lead exposure and the cognitive development of urban preschool children: the Cincinnati lead study cohort at age 4 years

    Neurotoxicol. Teratol.

    (1991)
  • K.N Dietrich et al.

    Lead exposure and the motor development status of urban six-year-old children in the Cincinnati Prospective Study

    Pediatrics

    (1993)
  • K.N Dietrich et al.

    The developmental consequences of low to moderate prenatal and postnatal lead exposure: intellectual attainment in the Cincinnati lead study cohort following school entry

    Neurotoxicol. Teratol.

    (1993)
  • K.N Dietrich et al.

    Symptomatic lead poisoning in infancy: a prospective case analysis

    J. Pediatr.

    (2000)
  • K.N Dietrich et al.

    Early exposure to lead and juvenile delinquency

    Neurotoxicol. Teratol.

    (2001)
  • L.M Dunn et al.

    PPVT Manual for Forms L and M

    (1981)
  • EPA et al.

    Report of the Clean Air Scientific Advisory Committee (CASAC): Review of the OAQPS and Staff Paper and the ECAO Air Quality Criteria Document Supplement, Report No. EPA-SAB-CASAC-90-002

    (1990)
  • D Faust et al.

    Moderately elevated blood lead levels: effects on neuropsychological functioning in children

    Pediatrics

    (1987)
  • D.M Fergusson et al.

    The effects of lead levels on the growth of word recognition in middle childhood

    Int. J. Epidemiol.

    (1993)
  • D.M Fergusson et al.

    Early dentine lead levels and subsequent cognitive and behavioral development

    J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry

    (1993)
  • M Fulton et al.

    Influence of blood lead on the ability and attainment of children in Edinburgh

    Lancet

    (1987)
  • P Grandjean et al.

    Lessons from a Danish study on neuropsychological impairment related to lead exposure

    Environ. Health Perspect.

    (1991)
  • O.N Hansen et al.

    A neuropsychological study of children with elevated dentine lead level: assessment of the effect of lead in different socio-economic groups

    Neurotoxicol. Teratol.

    (1989)
  • R.K Heaton

    Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual

    (1981)
  • A.B. Hollingshead, Four factor index of social status, Unpublished manuscript, Yale University,...
  • T.H Holmes et al.

    The social readjustment rating scale

    J. Psychosom. Res.

    (1967)
  • S.E Hyler

    Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire—4

    (1994)
  • S.W Jacobson

    Specificity of neurobehavioral outcomes associated with prenatal alcohol exposure

    Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res.

    (1998)
  • J.L Jacobson et al.

    Teratogenic effects of alcohol on infant development

    Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res.

    (1993)
  • S.W Jacobson et al.

    Prenatal alcohol exposure and infant information processing ability

    Child Dev.

    (1993)
  • Cited by (355)

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Requests for reprints should be sent to Lisa M. Chiodo, University Health Center, Suites 6D1 and 6E, 4201 St. Antoine, Detroit, MI 48201 or Sandra W. Jacobson, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Room 460, 2751 E. Jefferson, Detroit, MI 48207.

    View full text