Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Personality functioning: the influence of stature
Free
  1. F Ulph,
  2. P Betts,
  3. J Mulligan,
  4. R J Stratford
  1. University of Southampton, UK
  1. Correspondence to:
    Dr R Stratford
    Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Highfield, Southampton SO17 1BJ, UK; R.J.Stratfordsoton.ac.uk

Abstract

Background: The Wessex Growth Study has monitored the psychological development of a large cohort of short normal and average height control participants since school entry.

Aims: To examine the effect of stature on their personality functioning now that they are aged 18–20 years.

Methods: This report contains data from 48 short normal and 66 control participants. Mean height SD score at recruitment was: short normals −2.62 SD, controls −0.22 SD. Final height SD score was: short normals −1.86, controls 0.07. The Adolescent to Adult Personality Functioning Assessment (ADAPFA) measures functioning in six domains: education and employment, love relationships, friendships, coping, social contacts, and negotiations.

Results: No significant effect of recruitment height or final height was found on total ADAPFA score or on any of the domain scores. Socioeconomic status significantly affected total score, employment and education, and coping domain scores. Gender had a significant effect on total score, love relationships, coping, and social contacts domain scores. Salient aspects of daily living for this sample were identified from the interviews (prevalence%): consuming alcohol (94%), further education (63%), love relationships (55%), current drug use (29%), experience of violence (28%), parenthood (11%), and unemployment (9%). Stature was not significantly related to behaviour in any of these areas.

Conclusions: Despite previously reported links between short stature and poorer psychosocial adaptation, no evidence was found that stature per se significantly affected the functioning of the participants in these areas as young adults.

  • stature
  • transition to adulthood
  • young adult
  • personality functioning
  • ADAPFA, Adolescent to Adult Personality Functioning Assessment
  • APFA, Adult Personality Functioning Assessment
  • C, control
  • SES, socioeconomic status
  • SN, short normal

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

The possibility of treating short “normal” children following the creation of biosynthetic growth hormone in 1985 initiated the debate as to whether short stature per se constitutes a disability for which medical treatment is suitable.1–4 Treatment might be considered to be favourable if short stature was associated with poorer psychosocial adjustment, but without evidence suggesting this is so, any treatment must be seen as cosmetic.5

The psychosocial effects of short stature have been studied for decades producing a wealth of information concerning how short stature may affect children and adults.5–9 The literature suggests that adults of short stature are more likely to experience difficulties in the areas of education, employment, love relationships, and friendships.9–14 Findings, however, have been inconsistent and there is very little information relating to young adults. As young adulthood is possibly one of the most formative developmental phases, this lack of information gives reason for concern. In addition, much of the previous research has been flawed by methodological weaknesses, such as cross sectional designs, mixed diagnostic groups, and clinical samples.15

The Wessex Growth Study is a prospective longitudinal community based study that has followed the physical and psychosocial development of short normal participants and their average height controls from school entry.16 Results from previous phases of the study have challenged the perception that childhood short stature is associated with social and psychological disadvantage.17,18 In the current phase the participants were interviewed when they were aged between 18 and 20 years to assess the influence of childhood and adult height on personality functioning. The aims were to assess whether short stature identified at the age when height screening is now being recommended affects any of the developmental pathways to adulthood and to provide the growth specialist faced with a short normal child a contemporary commentary on psychological outcome.

METHODS

The participants in the Wessex Growth Study were initially recruited at school entry and have had height and weight measurements taken regularly since. Two previous reports have been made on psychological functioning at age 7–9 years16 and 11–13 years.18 This paper reports on assessments made when they were 18–20 years of age.

The participants were interviewed using a standard interview schedule—the Adolescent to Adult Personality Functioning Assessment (ADAPFA)—which measures social and interpersonal role performance in six domains: education and employment, love relationships, friendships, coping, social contacts, and negotiations.19 These are all developmental areas in which it has been shown in the literature that people with short stature may have difficulties. The domains are scored using an age related framework resulting in six domain scores between 0 and 5 with higher scores indicating poorer functioning. The domain scores can be aggregated to form a composite ADAPFA score, with a maximum of 30 and a cut off score of 16 above which functioning is regarded as dysfunctional.20 The ADAPFA is a development from the Adult Personality Functioning Assessment (APFA) which in research with adults has shown reliability and construct validity. ADAPFA, adapted to focus on the adolescent to adult transition, has been used in a recent follow up of interpersonal and social role performance in young people who experienced cancer in childhood, a study comparable in scope to the present one.

ADAPFA scoring, which provides information on the level of functioning within its six domains, is based on material from interviews lasting approximately an hour. Transcripts of interviews carried out in this study were further utilised to afford a more qualitative analysis of the participants’ life experience as emerging adults. This thematic analysis identified a set of discrete “marker” behaviours within each ADAPFA domain, which have been labelled collectively as “aspects of daily living” (see table 1). These relate to education received beyond school, employment status, relationships with a partner, parenthood, drug taking, drinking, and involvement with violence. Simple counts were made of the numbers of participants in each group who confirmed the behaviour during their interview.

Table 1

Derivation of aspects of daily living

Participants

At the beginning of this phase of the research 61% of the original participants were still available to the study (76 short normal (SN) and 94 control (C)). This reduction in sample size was due to attrition18 and an earlier recruitment of some of the participants into a separate study investigating the psychological effects of GH treatment.21 This treatment was offered to the very shortest of the total sample (less than −2 SD score for height), but allocation was random—by lot—leaving no systematic effect on the representativeness of the remainder. Of these remaining 170 participants, 114 (48 SN, 66 C) were interviewed (67%). Assessments were made to examine whether these 114 participants were representative of the available sample for interview (see table 2).

Table 2

comparison of interviewed participants with the remaining Wessex Growth Study sample

At initial recruitment, two distinct groups were selected: short normal participants, with height below the 2nd centile according to the 1990 UK Growth Standards22 and age and gender matched average height controls.17 During the course of the Wessex Growth Study there has been variation in participants’ height SD scores in both the short and average height control groups in some cases to such a degree that there is considerable overlap of the two groups’ final height SD scores (fig 1). The ADAPFA scores and the aspects of daily living results were therefore analysed to examine the effect of both recruitment and final height.

Figure 1

Height at final measurement for the participants involved in study in SD.

The effect of final height was examined by reallocating the participants into three height groups based on their final height centile: <2nd centile (n = 19), 2nd–50th centile (n = 61), and >50th centile (n = 34). Since the middle group (2nd–50th centile) consisted of both initial short normal and average height participants, the outcome variables for these participants were compared, and homogeneity was shown.

Analyses

To control for the potential effects of both gender25 and socioeconomic status (SES)23,24 on personality functioning,26,27 group mean differences in the total and six domain ADAPFA scores between height groups were examined using multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA). In this way, differences between the height groups associated with gender (with males typically taller), and with SES (with the more affluent groups typically taller) would be controlled for these two critical independent variables. Gender was established on entry to the study and SES information had previously been supplied by parents.18 (The data in the coping domain when adult height groups were separated reached significance (p = 0.005) on Levenes’ test of homogeneity, indicating that the data should not be analysed parametrically. A Kruskal-Wallis H test was used to analyse this data, and this comparison alone was not adjusted for gender and SES). Analyses of variance and χ2 tests were used to explore the possible mechanisms for significant class and gender effects on the ADAPFA domains. Patterns of behaviour in the aspects of daily living categories were compared using the χ2 test.

RESULTS

ADAPFA

Effect of recruitment height

Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations of the short normal and control group participants’ scores on total and six domains of ADAPFA. After adjusting for gender and SES, there was no significant difference between the groups selected on recruitment height on the ADAPFA derived scores (F = 1.281, df = 7, 104, p = 0.267). Thus recruitment height did not appear to be affecting how the participants functioned in adult roles in society. There were no significant univariate differences between the height groups after the adjustment. ADAPFA total scores, and three of the domain scores produced higher scores for short normal than average height participants, but these differences were slight, and none were significant when compared individually.

Table 3

Mean total and domain scores on ADAPFA for short normal and control groups, determined by childhood height, before adjustment for gender and social class

As expected, however, the covariates of gender and socioeconomic status both contributed to performance on the ADAPFA. Gender had a significant effect on the total ADAPFA score (F = 7.041, p = 0.009) and also on the love relationships (F = 4.13, p = 0.045), social contact (F = 4.115, p = 0.045), and coping domain scores. In each instance, males scored higher than females, indicating poorer functioning. It was thought that the relation between gender and social contact domain scores may be influenced by involvement in violence, as such behaviour would result in higher scoring and males more frequently reported being involved in violence (males 37%, females 17%, p = 0.064). The relation between involvement in violence and problems in the social contacts domain also approached significance (p = 0.061).

Socioeconomic status had a significant effect on total ADAPFA score (F = 14.304, p = 0.000) and also on the domains of education and employment (F = 11.199, p = 0.001), and coping. In each of these areas low socioeconomic status was associated with poorer functioning. The IQ of all participants had been obtained previously18 and since IQ is associated with social class and educational outcome, we examined the influence of IQ on the education and employment domain but found no significant effect (p = 0.13).

There were no significant interaction effects between height, class, and gender.

A similar number of short normal and control participants had a total ADAPFA score of 16 or above, which is taken as indicating some degree of personality dysfunction (SN: 10 (21%), C: 11 (17%), p = 0.371).

Effect of adult stature

Table 4 shows the means and standard deviations of the three adult height group participants’ scores on total and five of the six domains of ADAPFA. The MANCOVA results found no effect of adult height on personality functioning after adjusting for SES and gender (F = 0.884, df 14, 206, p = 0.518). Coping domain scores were not distributed appropriately for parametric analysis, but the Kruskal-Wallis score obtained for an unadjusted comparison was not significant either. There were no significant univariate differences between the height groups after the adjustment. Again, though, the mean total ADAPFA score was higher for the shortest adult height group, and domain scores for Friendship and Negotiation were also close to a significant difference when compared individually across the three height groups.

Table 4

Mean (SD) total and domain ADAPFA scores for the three groups based on adult height, before adjustment for gender and social class

The patterns of significant effects of gender and SES were however the same as those found when comparing the recruitment height groups. Gender had a significant effect on total ADAPFA score (F = 7.7, p = 0.006), love relationships (F = 3.861, p = 0.052), social contacts (F = 4.739, p = 0.039), and coping (p = 0.004). SES had a significant effect on total ADAPFA score (F = 14.304, p<0.001) and the education and employment domain score (F = 11.199, p = 0.001). Again the percentages of participants with ADAPFA scores above the suggested cut off point of 16 for psychological dysfunction were similar across the groups (<2 SD: 4 (21%), −2 to 0 SD: 11 (18%), >0 SD: 6 (18%), p = 0.948).

Aspects of daily living

No significant effects of either recruitment height or adult height were found on the aspects of daily living (tables 5 and 6).

Table 5

Comparisons of the percentage of short normal and control young people reporting behaviours labelled as “aspects of daily living”, based on height at initial recruitment

Table 6

Comparisons of the percentage of short average and above average height young people reporting behaviours labelled as “aspects of daily living”, based on final adult height)

DISCUSSION

In this study, childhood stature and final adult stature have not had a significant effect on the personality functioning of young adults. Though the shortest group has received slightly higher scores in some domains, the young people across the height groups interviewed have generally described similar patterns of behaviour. These results are in line with previous results from the Wessex Growth Study that short normal stature has not had a significant adverse effect on functioning during childhood and early adolescence.16–18

It has been suggested in the literature that adults of short stature might function differently in the areas of education, employment, love relationships, and friendships,9–14 but no significant differences in these areas were found. The Wessex Growth Study is the first longitudinal study of the effects of stature in a community sample and thus may show a truer picture of the effects of stature on daily living than previous reports which have predominantly been cross sectional, or based on mixed diagnostic group or clinic referred samples. The participants were identified solely on the basis of their height at school entry and none had been referred to a growth clinic, or had any concern expressed about them. Crucially also their age at recruitment reflected the time when treatment decisions (relating to short stature) are being made. The outcomes described in this paper therefore imply that an increase in height per se for these participants would not necessarily have contributed any more to their quality of life. Clearly there may still be referrals to growth clinics, but perhaps such clinics could review psychotherapeutic alternatives to medical treatment for those who do express height related concerns (usually on behalf of others: their children). In some cases psychological concerns might already exist before referral to a growth clinic, suggesting perhaps that a form of psychological screening might be relevant, though this is not a direct indication from the study.

The data from this study do, however, confirm that personality functioning is influenced by both gender and SES. The relation between personality and gender is not surprising. Costa et al have shown the cultural stability of male and female personality traits,27 and previous studies using the parent measure of the ADAPFA, the ADPFA, have reported gender effects.20 The females in our study were more likely than males to be functioning independently and to be in a love relationship and less likely to have problems in the social contacts domain. There are several explanations for these findings. First, even in European cultures such as ours, social role functioning is consistent with gender stereotypes with women still performing the majority of household tasks.28 It is possible that gender differences in the coping domain were attributable simply to the females’ greater involvement in this area as much of the rating in this domain rests on the participants’ ability to feed and clothe themselves and manage their finances. Second, the participants were assessed when they were 18–20 years old, the youngest age group for which the ADAPFA is recommended. It is possible that, in the domain of love relationships, a gender bias is inherent when used with such a group. Further studies including 18–20 year olds are needed to substantiate this hypothesis. Third, the domain of social contacts is influenced by involvement in violence. Reports of violent acts increase scores and in our study, males, short and control, tended to report involvement in acts of violence more often than the females.

The young adults in our study who were from a lower SES were more likely to experience problems in education, employment, and day to day living tasks such as managing finances. Others have also found that SES affects both personality functioning23 and education.24 IQ did not explain the relation between SES and the education and employment domain scores, possibly because participants with lower IQs were performing adequately in the employment domain. The finding that SES but not IQ was associated with scoring on the education and employment domain suggests that participants with lower SES were less likely to achieve their potential in these areas. Indeed research shows that lower childhood SES can have negative effects on later adult life.29

The results of this study seem to reflect untroubled development for short normal participants. However, it is of concern to note that a significant number of young adults, both short and control, reported involvement in high risk taking behaviour such as drug use and severe violence (table 5), and that 10 SN and 11 C participants showed some degree of personality dysfunction. Neither childhood nor adult short stature appears to be a contributory factor. While neither recruitment or adult height can be seen as a contributory factor, further analysis of the specific determinants here will be reported separately. Similarly the interaction of pubertal timing with these life experiences is also of interest, perhaps particularly in a study of growth, and again possible relations here are being examined.

Some limitations to the findings of the present study are evident. Firstly, as described in the methods section of the 170 participants who remained available for this phase of the study, only 114 participants could be interviewed. These participants were however found to be representative of the total sample available for this phase of the research.

Secondly, the height SD of a proportion of the short normal participants is now above the original centile band defining short stature. Such a phenomenon has been reported in other studies.30 Few, however, had a height above the 25th centile and our results are from a sample of young adults who for the majority of their lives have been shorter than their peers, having been recruited at the critical age for treatment decisions.

In summary, no significant differences in personality functioning or aspects of daily living were found which could be attributable to height. This should not be interpreted as indicating that people with short stature will not experience problems in their development, but that they are no more likely to do so than those who are taller. This study is unique as it reports on the effect of both childhood height on adult functioning and the effect of adult height on functioning in the same sample

REFERENCES

Linked Articles

  • Atoms
    Howard Bauchner