Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Family conflict and slow growth

Abstract

AIMS Having previously observed that slow growth in childhood is associated with subsequent labour market disadvantage, an attempt was made to determine whether family conflict is associated with slow growth to age 7 years, independently of material disadvantage.

METHODS A total of 6574 children born between 3 and 9 March 1958 who were members of the British National Child Development Study were used in these analyses. Slow growth at age 7 years was indicated by short stature defined as the lowest fifth of the height distribution. In multivariate analysis, adjustment was made for fully attained adult height as a measure of genetically predetermined height.

RESULTS A total of 31.1% of children who had experienced family conflict were of short stature compared with 20.2% of those who had not, representing relative odds of 1.79 (95% confidence interval (CI) 1.39 to 2.30). After adjustment for social class, crowding, sex, and predetermined height, the relative odds were slightly reduced to 1.62 (95% CI 1.18 to 2.23). A total of 44.0% of children from the most crowded households were of short stature compared with 16.4% of those from the least crowded. The unadjusted relative odds were 3.99 (95% CI 2.94 to 5.41) and after adjustment for the potential confounding variables they were 3.07 (95% CI 2.08 to 4.51). Low social class was also a risk for short stature at age 7 years, but this was not statistically significant after adjustment for the other confounding factors.

CONCLUSIONS Family conflict during childhood was independently associated with slow growth to age 7 years.

  • Stress caused by family conflict results in slow growth to age 7 years

  • A higher proportion of the shortest 20% of 7 year old children have experienced psychosocial stress

  • Slow growth may indicate impaired psychological development caused by stress

  • Family conflict may have lifelong consequences for some children

  • Slow growth may be a useful marker for psychosocial stress

  • growth
  • family conflict
  • stress
  • development

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

There have been previous reports of psychosocial and material influences on height and growth.1-3 We decided to investigate evidence of the effect of psychosocial and material circumstances on slow growth in childhood among members of the National Child Development Study (NCDS): the 1958 British birth cohort.

Longevity and upward social mobility are strongly and independently related to height and height appears to be an important indicator of social variation in health.4 The strength of the relation between height and social mobility led to the heights of civil servants in the Whitehall Study being more closely related to achieved occupational grade than to their social class of origin.5It was once widely assumed that findings such as these indicated that height was a marker for some physiological advantage and also perhaps that tall people were upwardly mobile because of a human tendency to be impressed by stature. These interpretations are unlikely to represent a full explanation, however, as it has been shown that unemployment in early adulthood, which is a good marker for general labour market disadvantage, was much more closely related to slowed growth to age 7 years than to fully attained adult height.6 This suggests that some feature of the childhood environment may influence both early growth rate and labour market success.

If slow growth is identified as an indicator of psychosocial stress caused by family conflict, this may provide more information about the processes by which more health risks are accumulated across the life course among those who experience adversity in childhood. As growth reflects underlying metabolic activity, slow growth could also be a marker for processes that are themselves potentially damaging for future mental and physical health.

Subjects and methods

SUBJECTS

The data used were taken from the NCDS,7-10 a continuing national longitudinal study of approximately 16 000 subjects living in Great Britain who were born between 3 and 9 March 1958. There have been six data collection sweeps at birth and at ages 7, 11, 16, 23, and 33 years. As a result of sample attrition and the exclusion of subjects with incomplete data, these analyses were limited to 3169 male and 3405 female subjects. The cohort has nevertheless remained largely representative, although the most disadvantaged groups are under-represented.11

MEASURES

Height at age 7 years was measured by school doctors to the nearest inch. The bottom fifth of the height distribution (estimated separately for boys and girls), containing the shortest children, was used as a binary variable in the analysis. The shortest fifth was chosen as a possible indicator of slowed growth for two reasons: firstly, because a similar measure used in previous analyses6 clearly identified a group who would subsequently experience a significantly increased risk of labour market disadvantage in adult life, even after adjustment for the relevant potential confounding factors; and, secondly, by focusing on the shortest fifth of children we are attempting to show that we have defined a group that may be readily identified as containing a higher proportion of children who have experienced psychosocial stress. Height at age 33 years was measured to the nearest centimetre by social research interviewers using portable stadiometers. Height at age 33 years was used as a continuous variable. Where heights were defined as a group variable, the proportions in each group vary slightly as a function of rounding bias and sex standardisation.

A binary measure of family conflict was taken from a questionnaire completed by the cohort member’s health visitor at age 7 years. The health visitor reported family difficulties due to domestic tension, divorce, separation, or desertion. This was based on the health visitor’s previous knowledge of the family, usually through involvement of the family with statutory or voluntary bodies such as the Marriage Guidance Council.

The Registrar General’s social class classification based on the father’s occupation when the cohort member was 7 years of age was used to indicate socioeconomic conditions. Subjects were excluded where the father’s occupation was not recorded, including where the father was not resident in the household. Crowding was used as an indicator of social disadvantage12 to provide a more accurate measure of the standard of living13 than social class alone. The measure of crowding was based on the number of people per room, excluding kitchens and bathrooms, when the cohort members were aged 7 years. This was divided into four categories: up to one person per room; more than one and up to one and a half people per room; more than one and a half and up to two people per room; and more than two people per room. The last category represents the greatest relative disadvantage.

STATISTICAL METHODS

The relation between family conflict, family circumstances, and growth to age 7 years was examined by cross tabulation (statistical significance was established using the χ2 test for trend) and multiple logistic regression. Height at age 7 years was divided at the bottom fifth to form a binary dependent variable. We adjusted for height at 33 years, modelled as a linear variable, in the multiple logistic regression model. This model was also adjusted for social class, crowding, and sex. All independent variables, with the exception of adult height at age 33 years, were modelled using binary dummies. Subjects with incomplete data were excluded from the analysis. All analyses were conducted using SPSS.14

Results

A total of 299 (4.5%) cohort members had been exposed to family conflict by age 7 years as indicated by the health visitor’s report. The height range at age 7 years for boys in the shortest fifth was 1.02–1.17 m, with a mean of 1.15 m. The range for other boys was 1.19–1.52 m, with a mean of 1.25 m. For short girls the range was 0.99–1.17 m, with a mean of 1.14 m; for the taller girls the range was 1.19–1.55 m with a mean of 1.24 m. A total of 1358 boys and girls were in the short stature group at age 7 years, defined by the bottom fifth of the sex standardised height distribution.

Of those children who experienced family conflict, 31.1% were in the bottom fifth of the height distribution at age 7 years, compared with 20.2% of those who did not experience family conflict (table 1). The children who experienced family conflict were also less likely to be in the tallest height group at age 7 years: 14.4% compared with 20.9% of those who had not reported conflict by age 7 years. This relation was highly statistically significant (p < 0.0005). Table 2 shows that the relation between height at age 33 years and the measure of family conflict in earlier life is much weaker (p = 0.051). The relation remains in the same direction as for height measured in childhood, however, with 23.7% of those who experienced family conflict being in the shortest height group at age 33 years, compared with 18.9% of those who did not.

Table 1

Height distribution at age 7 years by family conflict. Values are percentages

Table 2

Height distribution at age 33 years by family conflict. Values are percentages

Table 3 has short stature at age 7 years (defined by the bottom fifth of the height distribution6) as the outcome variable. The relative odds of being in the lowest fifth of the height distribution among those who experienced family conflict were 1.79 (95% confidence interval (CI) 1.39 to 2.30) when compared with those who had not experienced family conflict. After adjustment for the potential confounding variables the relative odds were slightly reduced to 1.62 (95% CI 1.18 to 2.23), indicating a relation between slow growth and family conflict which was independent of final adult height and the measures of cultural and material circumstances in childhood.

Table 3

Slow growth at age 7 years indicated by the shortest fifth of the height distribution. Relative odds are adjusted for domestic conflict, social class, crowding, sex, and adult height

In social class V, 30.3% of children were in the short stature group, compared with 11.7% in class I, with relative odds of 3.29 (95% CI 2.25 to 4.82). After adjustment for the potential confounding variables the strength of this relation was significantly decreased and the relative odds for short stature among children from social class V families were reduced to 1.45 (95% CI 0.90 to 2.32) when compared with children from class I. Thus after adjustment, the relation between social class and height at age 7 years was no longer statistically significant.

There was a clear gradient of association between crowding and short stature at age 7 years. A total of 44.0% of those from the most crowded households were of short stature, whereas only 16.4% of those from the least crowded households were in the shortest height fifth. The unadjusted relative odds for being in the bottom height fifth for those in the most crowded households were 3.99 (95% CI 2.94 to 5.41) compared with those from the least crowded households. Adjustment for the potential confounding variables slightly reduced these relative odds to 3.07 (95% CI 2.08 to 4.51).

Discussion

It has previously been observed that 7 year old boys with slowed growth are far more likely to experience later adult unemployment, even after adjustment for socioeconomic background, educational attainment, and other risk factors for unemployment.6 It is hypothesised that environmental factors, including family conflict, have dual consequences: slowing growth and also affecting psychological development. The psychological consequences of this can result in labour market disadvantage and socioeconomic adversity in adult life. Although those with slowed growth in childhood do not necessarily become the shortest adults, their poorer psychological development may result in subsequent labour market and other disadvantages. This paper has focused on psychosocial influences, in the form of family conflict, on growth rate to age 7 years. We have attempted to adjust for genetically determined height (by adjusting for fully attained adult height) and have thus focused on slowed growth.

A child’s height at age 7 years is the result of the interaction between genetic and environmental influences.15 In a study of 7569 English and Scottish primary schoolchildren, Smith et al found that the socioeconomic differences in height are established in early childhood.16 It is known, however, that only a relatively small proportion of those of short stature at age 7 years are of short stature as adults,17 indicating a significant variation in growth rates. Here, we are concerned with slowed growth to age 7 years as this may be a better indicator of environmental effects than persistent short stature. Our analysis is likely to have underestimated the influence of family conflict and socioeconomic circumstances on growth in childhood. This is because we adjusted for fully attained adult height: family conflict and socioeconomic circumstances may also result in shorter fully attained adult height.18 This ‘over adjustment’ was necessary to identify slowed growth rather than genetically determined short stature. The measure of family conflict used here was only taken at one time point and gave no indication of the severity or duration of exposure to conflict. As the measure of conflict was based on the health visitor’s prior knowledge of the family and their use of relevant statutory and voluntary services, many cases of conflict will have remained unreported. There is likely to be a considerable amount of unobserved heterogeneity resulting in a weakening of the association between conflict and slowed growth reported here.

Family conflict before the age of 7 years had a strong and statistically significantly association with slow growth to age 7 years, even after adjustment for the potential confounding factors. The same measure of conflict was only weakly associated with fully attained adult height. Our results show independent relations of slower growth in childhood with family conflict and crowding. Lower parental social class was associated with short stature at age 7 years in the unadjusted model, but the significance of this relation was considerably reduced by adjustment for family conflict and crowding. Social class is an indicator of parental characteristics, such as level of education, as well as cultural and material circumstances in the home. The weak relation of social class with slowed growth in the adjusted model indicates that family conflict itself may influence development and that the measure of family conflict is not simply an indicator of other harmful family characteristics. Inclusion in the multivariate model of other factors known to be associated with childhood height, such as birth weight and parental smoking habits, did not significantly affect the relation of family conflict with height at age 7 years (data not shown). This confirms the finding from Swedish data, based only on retrospective recall, that height is related independently to economic hardship in childhood and to ‘family dissension’.3 It is plausible that such influences are likely to affect emotional and physical development. These developmental consequences may themselves affect health and social mobility later in life.

The relation between household crowding and the risk of slowed growth at age 7 years is powerful and graded. This may be because crowding is a good indicator of low income and poor housing conditions and this is reflected in slower childhood growth. Frequent sleep disturbance may be another mechanism through which growth is affected in crowded households. Growth hormone is released during periods of deep sleep and its secretion rate could fall if sleep were disrupted.19The secretion of other hormones required for normal physical development is also stimulated by growth hormone.20Conditions that are more likely to result in disturbed sleep, such as excessive noise, shared rooms, or shared beds, could reduce growth hormone secretion and may result in slowed growth.

Family conflict during childhood has been shown to be a risk factor for subsequent psychological ill health21 and it has also been suggested that slow growth in infancy may be associated with adult depression.22 Factors affecting growth rate may influence future physical health as slow growth in infancy has been associated with an increased risk of adult coronary heart disease.23Psychosocial stress may be responsible for these processes.

The mechanisms by which stress can influence growth and health risks are not fully understood, but basal levels of growth hormone were found to be reduced in young rhesus monkeys as a response to chronic psychological stress, probably due to a shift in the hypothalamic regulatory process.24 Possible pathways include stress induced by persistent family conflict increasing levels of the endogenous opioid, β endorphin.25 This has been shown to modulate growth hormone release through the mediation of growth hormone releasing hormone in prepubertal children.26

The chronic childhood stress resulting from family conflict that is associated with slowed growth may have implications for future cognitive ability. Significant hippocampal degeneration has been observed in captive vervet monkeys due to increased glucocorticoid levels resulting from chronic social stress.27 If stressed children suffered hippocampal damage, then, eventually, their cognitive ability would suffer, as the hippocampus is important in learning and memory. The effect of this damage can be observed in rats, which are less able to run through mazes if there has been damage to the hippocampus.28

Stress, indicated by slow growth, could also directly increase the risk of physical illness. Studies of baboons have shown that the repeated stress response (resulting in slower human growth) significantly increases cardiovascular risk factors.29 This process may begin in childhood, but the accumulation of significant cardiovascular risk is likely to result from many years of stress. Other more immediate consequences of chronic childhood stress may be due to suppression of the immune system, increasing the risk of acute infection.30

Adverse childhood conditions may have dual risks for both poor future health and persistent socioeconomic disadvantage, which is itself a risk factor for poor health. Domestic conflict is known to be associated with a number of adverse long term developmental, physical health, psychological,31-33 and labour market6 outcomes. It may be that family conflict begins a spiral of disadvantage that results in the accumulation of health risks throughout life. A relatively high proportion of prepuberatal children in the shortest height fifth are likely to have experienced significant psychosocial adversity due to family conflict. These children are also more likely to experience socioeconomic disadvantages in adult life.

Conclusions

Slow growth in childhood is associated with family conflict and this is independent of socioeconomic circumstances. This may be because slow growth is a sensitive marker of emotional disturbance and chronic stress in childhood. The association of slow growth in childhood with labour market disadvantage in later life may be due to childhood psychosocial and material adversity resulting in adverse psychological and cognitive outcomes. Slow growth may be a useful marker for psychosocial stress.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for assistance from the staff of the Social Statistics Research Unit, City University, London, who are responsible for the National Child Development Study.

References